new internationalist 119January 1983
Light fantastic HOUR AFTER HOUR, day
after day, the images flicker out. The programmes may change but the
attention scarcely wavers. What is it about television that can mesmerise most
of the Western world'? Why is the box addictive? Australian researchers
Fred and Merrelyn Emery claim to
have hit upon the answer. Forget the programmes. they
say. Whether it's the 'Ascent of Man' or a Bugs
Bunny cartoon makes no difference at all. The important thing is the special
kind of light that the TV emits. This is what stuns the brain cells and
closes down the mind. The glow from your TV
screen, they say, is difficult for the human nervous system to cope with.
This is firstly because it is 'radiant' rather than 'ambient' light and
secondly because it is constantly switching on and off. This is different from
what we are used to. Human beings usually absorb
information via 'ambient' light - light that has been
reflected off objects. Light that started out from the sun or a light
bulb is being reflected from the white spaces
between the letters on this page and then travelling on into your eye. But if you look now at
the light source itself, you get radiant light at a much higher intensity. And this is something, say the Emerys, that the human
brain cannot make much use of. 'The human perceptual system,' they say. 'evolved to deal with ambient light, not radiant light'.
Nothing in evolution has prepared us for the luminous beams from the
television set. Our ancestors' only similar experience might have been star-gazing or staring into the fire. And
since we have no evolved system for extracting information from radiant
light, 'We don't try to do it. We cut off'. But TV light, they say,
has a second important characteristic that sets it aside from other human
experience. It is pulsating very
rapidly and regularly - fifty or sixty times per second - (see box). This
could produce 'habituation' - the brain gets used to the rhythm of the rapid
changes and becomes so fixated by them that the picture itself fades into
insignificance. Television, they claim, can only be seen
as a 'direct technological analogue of the hypnotist', with the brain
effectively dominated by the signal. 'Provided the viewers continue to watch,
they are unlikely to reflect on what they are viewing.' The Emery's first put
forward this startling hypnothesis on the physiological effect of TV back in
1975 while they were at the Australian National University of Canberra. Then
in 1978 they appeared before a government committee enquiring into the
effects of television on children and which recommended the 'priority be
given to testing their theories'. In fact
relatively little was done; for all their efforts the Emerys found it almost
impossible to get funds. Most of their
conclusions are based on experiments done by other
people in related fields. These have usually involved assessing 'brain waves;
- those electronic pulses which can be measured by tapping electrodes to the
human scalp. The waves are of two main types. Alpha waves correspond to when
the brain is relaxing and not processing information while the faster beta
waves are produced when the brain is actively organising an analysing what it
receives. Dr Herbert E. Krugman
of the US General Electric Company, for example, has compared the brain waves
produces when reading magazines with those produced when watching television.
Magazine reading produced largely beta waves while watching television for
just 30 seconds produced a 'characteristic mode of response' with a
predominance of the slower alpha waves - regardless of the programme. Another experiment in
the That their most avid
viewers are 'spaced out' a good deal of the time raises some awkward
questions for the producers of television programmes. As Dr Krugman put it in
a recent paper for the US Journal of Advertising Research: 'Students of media
behaviour may yet confront the embarrassing fact that television audiences
give close attention for long periods of time to stimulii that create no
thought and very little recall'. Like the Emery's, Dr
Krugman is concerned to find out what the brain is doing all this time if it
is not actively working. His approach is to look specifically at the distinction
between left-brain and right-brain activity. It is
now accepted that the left side of the brain is used for organising
and analysing information, whereas the right has much more general functions.
Studies at the Department of Psychology at Krugman suggests that
it is the right-brain that maintains the vigil over
the tolerance of the TV screen and which only nudges the left-brain into
alertness as needed. The tolerance of the right-brain
for sustained attention is what accounts for childrens' ability to watch TV
for hours on end. 'We should find nothing remarkable about this
physiologically. 'he says. 'It is not more
remarkable than the ability of a truck driver to drive his vehicle for many
hours and to keep adequate watch on the road ahead. 'Of course both
children and truck drivers may have to fight to stay awake because of the
hypnotic monotony of the situation. This is not because their brain is
working hard but because it is working very little.' The remarkable aspect
of the Emerys' proposition is that it provides a physiological reason -
beyond that of boredom or fatigue —why the brain might be closing down. And, if it is the radiant, repetitive light signal that is
doing the damage, there is not much that the makers of programmes or
commercials can do about it. Indeed the
implications are so serious that you would have expected a whole flood of
research programmes designed to test the proposition. In fact there has been
almost nothing, The Emerys' themselves, who are now working at the University
of Pennsylvania in the United States, have been trying for years to get the
finances to back a major study. 'We have had
neurophysiologists standing by and research proposals in for as little as $11,000,' says Merrelyn Emery, 'but no one wants to fund
them. My suspicion is that there are a lot of people terrified that we may
well be right.' The implications for
the makers of educational programmes on television would be radical. Fred
Emery is particularly forthright. Giving evidence before the Australian
government commission, he said: 'If we are going to
show TV in school hours then we have to be very clear about what we are
doing: we are wasting our time.' And if it is the
fluctuating light that is closing down the brain then there are implications
possibly even more serious. The Emerys suggest that those parts of the brain
that are being closed down are the ones that
normally exert control over more basic instincts. Without such control the
old or primitive brain, which we share with lower order species, has much
more of a free rein and long-term viewing could produce anything from
irritability to aggression. Violent behaviour that results from watching
television may thus come not from the programmes but from the medium itself. It is clear however
that the 'habituation' effect that the TV screen has cannot be totally effective. People who are viewing screens in an
active way such as those reading visual display units on computers are
obviously managing to overcome to a great extent whatever
effect there is. In the first place they are moving
their eyes to read, whereas the TV viewer tends to sit far enough back so
that almost no eye movement is needed to take in the screen. Then again, as
with reading a magazine or a book, the brain is constantly refreshed and
rested by looking away or choosing another piece of information to be studied. Herbert Krugman' s conclusion from the evidence that has been
gathered is that educational TV will have to work much harder to imitate the
action of reading or of being in the presence of a good teacher. To actually get people to think while they are watching,
he says, the producers should be much more tolerant of pauses - rest stops
for the tired brain. Timing is everything, That people are not
actively thinking as they are viewing does not, however, mean that the images
are not going into - and being retained by - the brain. It is
generally accepted that politics, for example, has become much more a
matter of TV image than argument. That the electorate in the The same would be true
for advertising. As Fred Emery puts it: 'Television is very
good at familiarising people with something. Hence
its great value in advertising - especially for launching new products. It
gets over the strangeness straightaway'. Again in
news or current affairs broadcasts the likelihood is that people have become
more familiar with the sights of But when it comes to the mechanism of learning
from the television screen, we still seem, after all these years, to be
in fairly unknown territory.
|